Peer Review Version The different impact of attention, movement and sensory information on body metric representation Journal: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Manuscript ID QJE-STD-22-414.R1 Manuscript Type: Standard Article Date Submitted by the Author: 15-May-2023 Complete List of Authors: Caggiano, Pietro; University of Hertfordshire, Cocchini, Gianna; Goldsmiths University of London Department of Psychology, De Stefano, Danila; Goldsmiths University of London Romano, Daniele; University of Milan at Bicocca Keywords: body representation, body schema, body image, body metric, arm bisection task Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version The different impact of attention, movement and sensory information on body metric representation Pietro Caggiano1#, Gianna Cocchini2, Danila De Stefano2, Daniele Romano3 1 School of Life & Medical Sciences, University of Hetfordshire, Hatfield, UK 2 Psychology Department, Goldsmiths University of London, New Cross, London, UK 3 Psychology Department, University of Milano Bicocca, Milan, Italy #Corresponding author: Pietro Caggiano https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5560-4870 University of Hertfordshire Hatfield AL10 9AB UK p.caggiano@herts.ac.uk Page 1 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version The different impact of attention, movement and sensory information on body metric representation Abstract A growing body of research investigating the relationship between body representation and tool-use has shown that body representation is highly malleable. The nature of the body representation does not consist only of sensory attributes but also of motor action-oriented qualities, which may modulate the subjective experience of our own body. However, how these multisensory factors and integrations may specifically guide and constrain body reorientation’s plasticity has been under-investigated. In the present study, we used a forearm bisection task to selectively investigate the contribution of motor, sensory and attentional aspects in guiding body representation malleability. Results show that the perceived forearm midpoint deviates from the real one. This shift is further modulated by a motor task but not by a sensory task, whereas the attentional task generates more uncertain results. Our findings provide novel insight into the individual role of movement, somatosensation and attention in modulating body metric representation. Keywords: body representation, body schema, body image, body metric, arm bisection task. Page 2 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version Introduction How we represent our own body is crucial for everyday actions and for successful interactions with the external environment (Maravita et al., 2003). Although it may appear a fairly simple concept to grasp, the understanding of the underlying cognitive processes and the interaction of various factors is complex and convoluted. Traditionally, the terms body schema and body image have been extensively used to conceptualise one’s own body representation (Gallagher, 1986; 2005); however, different representations have emerged at different times throughout the literature and, despit numerous studies have been dedicated to investigating body representations, a clear-cut theory or model providing a full explanation of the relationships between the different bodily representations remains difficult to delineate (e.g., de Vignemont, 2010; Longo, 2022). Although the notion of body representation is in itself yet to be unanimously characterised, current literature does offer a critical and reliable body of evidence indicating that body representation is highly plastic and malleable (e.g., Martel et al., 2016, 2021; Medina & Coslett, 2010; Caggiano et al., 2021). and can be affected by pathological conditions, such as acquired brain damage (e.g. Bassolino et. al, 2022; Caggiano et al., 2020; Tosi et al., 2018; Garbarini et al., 2015; Mora et al., 2023) and limb amputation (e.g Rognini et al., 2019; Sato et al., 2017; Canzoneri et al., 2013). In healthy population, the plasticity and reorganisation of metric body representation has been investigated in the context of ageing (Sorrentino et al., 2021 ; Garbarini et al., 2015) gestures (Mora et al., 2021), passive and active actions (Bruno et al., 2019), bodily illusions (Tosi et al., 2022), response modality (Tosi et al., 2020) and tool-use where the morphology and functional aspects of a tool appear to modulate the subjective experience of our own body metrics (e.g., Maravita & Romano, 2018; Galigani et al., 2020; Romano et al., 2019). Manipulation of tools re-shapes the body representation during and after use (Martel et al., 2016) as observed in cases of tool embodiment (e.g., Iriki et al., 1996; Maravita et al., 2002; Farnè et al., 2007; Maravita Page 3 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version & Iriki, 2004; Cardinali et al., 2009; Sposito et al., 2012; Cardinali et al., 2016). It seems, therefore, that the continuous reshaping of our perception of body parts is determined by various factors. Somatosensory attributes are clearly crucial components in the context of body plasticity, as indicated by a number of studies on tool embodiment. For example, in a recent study Martel and colleagues (2019) have shown that somatosensory signals evoked by tool use alone are sufficient to alter kinematic profiles of reach-to-grasp movements. Similarly, previous studies with brain-damaged patients with unilateral spatial attentional disorders demonstrated that tool- use could lead to crossmodal tactile extinction when a visual stimulus was presented at the extremity of a used (embodied) tool (Farnè et al., 2005, 2007; Farnè & Làdavas, 2000; Maravita et al., 2001), suggesting a predominance of sensory-motor information in determining embodiment and body representation plasticity. A hypothesis that has been recently challenged (Maravita & Romano, 2018). For example, in a recent study (Romano et al., 2019), we observed that active training, requiring movements either of the shoulders (proximal actions) or of the wrist (distal actions), induced a different shift of the perceived forearm’s midpoint. Crucially, the findings suggested that the type of action required while using a tool significantly contributes to modulating the representation of the body part involved. The tool-use-dependent change of body representation would not be a mere morphological change. A further crucial factor that tends to conflate with the motor aspect is attention, as the focus of attention tends to be directed to the part of the body that needs to be moved. It seems, therefore, that also shift of attentional resources can conflate with motor and sensory information (Holmes et al., 2007a; Homes et al., 2007b). Therefore, the representation of one’s own body part does not depend on one unitary mechanism but on a combination of factors. The aim of the current study is to investigate the Page 4 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version selective impact of motor and sensory aspects and focus of attention on body metric representation by means of a forearm bisection task. Materials and methods Participants A group of 24 right-handed healthy female adults took part in the study. Their average age was 25.7 years (SD = 3.73) and their average formal education was 17.1 years (SD = 2.9; range: 8- 22). The study was approved by the Goldsmiths Departmental Ethics Committee in accordance with the standards of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki (BMJ 1991; 302: 1194). All participants gave written informed consent before taking part in the study. Procedure Participants were blindfolded for the entire experiment and comfortably sat at a table in a floodlit and sound-attenuated room. Their right forearm rested, palm down, on the table in a comfortable position. Participants were asked to perform the Forearm Bisection Task, an experimental procedure frequently used in the context of body-metric representation studies (Garbarini et al., 2015; Romano et al., 2019; Sposito et al., 2010; 2012; Tosi et al., 2018; D’Angelo et al., 2018). In this task, participants are asked to indicate, with their left index finger, the midpoint of a body segment that goes from the tip of the middle finger to the right olecranon (elbow). Ballistic movements of the left-hand index finger indicated the subjective midpoint; corrections were not allowed. Each trial started with the left index finger placed at about 30 cm distance from the participant’s midsagittal plane in a standard point. To avoid tactile feedback by touching the right forearm during the bisection task, a custom-made plastic table ruler was placed a few millimetres above the forearm to be bisected. Once the left finger touched the plastic table ruler, it remained in place for a few seconds, allowing the experimenter Page 5 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version to record the position (subjective midpoint). Then the left finger was positioned back at the starting point for the new trial. Individual’s forearm length (i.e., distance from the elbow to the middle fingertip) was measured at the beginning of the study to calculate the objective forearm midpoint. The zero was set at the most proximal landmark (i.e., elbow). For each participant, the subjective midpoint was calculated by averaging each pointing position across trials (details on the number of trials and conditions are reported in the following sections below). We than calculated the subjective midpoint deviation as the difference between the perceived and objective midpoint, as a percentage of the participant’s actual forearm length: 𝑆𝑢𝑏𝑗𝑒𝑐𝑡𝑖𝑣𝑒 𝑀𝑖𝑑𝑝𝑜𝑖𝑛𝑡 𝐷𝑒𝑣𝑖𝑎𝑡𝑖𝑜𝑛 = (𝑆𝑢𝑏𝑗𝑒𝑐𝑡𝑖𝑣𝑒 𝑚𝑖𝑑𝑝𝑜𝑖𝑛𝑡 ― 𝑂𝑏𝑗𝑒𝑐𝑡𝑖𝑣𝑒 𝑚𝑖𝑑𝑝𝑜𝑖𝑛𝑡𝐹𝑜𝑟𝑒𝑎𝑟𝑚 𝑙𝑒𝑛𝑡ℎ ) × 100 According to this formula, a negative value indicates a proximal subjective deviation (i.e. a shift of the subjective midpoint toward the elbow), a positive value indicates a distal subjective deviation (i.e. a shift of the subjective midpoint toward the wrist) and a value equal to 0% indicates no deviation (i.e. subjective and objective midpoints are identical). Each participant performed the bisections under three different tasks respectively named: Motor, Sensory, and Attention (described below). The order of the tasks was counterbalanced across participants. Motor Task Participants were asked to perform the Forearm Bisection Task immediately after a movement of their right middle finger (Distal movement) or their right elbow (Proximal movement). Movements consisted of three relatively rapid taps of the finger or the elbow, maintaining the rest of the forearm as stationary as possible. Each movement condition consists of a block of 12 trials with no movement (baseline) followed by 12 trials with movement (either distal or proximal). Each participant performed both movement conditions for a total of 48 trials: 24 Page 6 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version for the two baselines and 24 for the two movement conditions. The order of movement conditions was counterbalanced across participants. Sensory task Participants were asked to perform the Forearm Bisection Task immediately after a somatosensory stimulation of either their right middle finger (Distal stimulation) or their right elbow (Proximal Stimulation) or both locations as an additional control condition (Distal- Proximal Stimulation). Double stimulation was introduced to distinguish the sensory effect from the possible attentional effect. Supra-threshold somatosensory stimulation (Francis et al., 2000) was induced by means of a vibration stimulator. Two vibration pads (10mm approximately) were placed on both the elbow and the middle finger. Three keys were used to deliver single stimulation either to the elbow or the middle finger, and double stimulation to both sites (i.e. the elbow and the finger) simultaneously. Vibration frequencies of 50Hz were delivered by the examiner for approximately 1 second and consisted of a block of 12 trials with no stimulation (baseline) followed by 12 trials of stimulation (either distal, proximal, or double). Each participant performed all the stimulation conditions (order counterbalanced across participants) for a total of 72 trials (i.e. 36 for the three baselines and 36 for three stimulation conditions). It should be noted that vibrotactile stimulation at the level of the elbow has been reported to induce illusions of limb movement. However, the frequency (50 Hz) and duration of stimulation (∼1 second) delivered in the current study did not induce any illusory movement as typically higher frequencies (above 90 Hz) and longer stimulation (above 100 seconds) are needed for vibration-induced illusions to occur (e.g. Purcell et al., 2020; Burrack & Brugger, 2005; de Vignemont et al., 2005; Lackner, 1988). Page 7 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version Attention task Participants were asked to perform the Forearm Bisection Task (as described above) immediately after focusing their attention on their hand (Distal attention) or on their elbow (Proximal attention). Location of attention was manipulated by asking participants to detect ‘very light’ somatosensory stimulation (i.e., vibrations) under the tip of their middle finger or under their elbow. The same equipment described above was used to deliver the vibrations. Participants were told that, in this task, the vibrations could be ‘extremely’ light and possibly under the individual threshold. They were also told that intensity increased across 6 trials until they could feel it. In reality, during the first five trials, the examiner pretended to deliver the stimulation, but no vibration was given. Under this condition, the participants would have focused their attention on the ‘stimulated’ extremity of their upper limb without interference from sensory information. A supra-threshold stimulation was delivered only on trial 6 to facilitate participants’ engagement on the task, and data from these trials were not considered in the final analysis. After each trial, they were asked to perform the arm bisection and asked whether they detected the vibration. Each attention condition (Distal or Proximal) consisted of two blocks of 12 trials without stimulation (baseline) followed by four blocks of 6 trials with distal or proximal stimulations. In total, each participant performed a total of 72 trials: 24 trials for the two baselines, 48 trials for proximal and distal attention stimulations; of these last trials, only those with ‘simulated stimulations’ (n=40) were entered in the final analysis. Statistical analysis Inferential statistics were performed through linear mixed models (LMM) by means of lme4 package implemented in the statistical software R (R Core Team 2016). Subjective Midpoint Deviation (SDM) was used as the dependent variable (i.e. the discrepancy between the perceived and actual midpoint between the elbow and the tip of the middle finger Page 8 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version calculated as indicated in the Procedure section), including subjects as the random intercept variable. The fixed effect models tested always included a within-subject factor Time with two levels (baseline/post manipulation) and another within-subjects factor, Body Part. The latter has two levels for the motor and attention tasks (distal/proximal), and three levels for the sensory task (distal/proximal/both). Significant interactions have been explored by the inspection of confidence intervals of the different conditions for which we reported mean values and 95% Confidence Intervals (CI) (Cohen, 1990, 1994; Masson & Loftus, 2003). Results Motor Task LMM analysis revealed a significant main effect of Time (F(1,1020)=17.719, p<.001) and Body Part (F(1,1020)=11.972, p<.001). The interaction was not significant (F(1,1020)=0.074, p=.786). The inspection of CI (Figure 1) shows that moving a body part induces a proximal shift (baseline: -4.25% [-7.34,-1.16]; post-action: -5.32% [-8.42,-2.23]). Moreover, the block involving the elbow shows a more proximal deviation than those involving the index finger (proximal: -5.23% [-8.32,-2.14]; distal: -4.35% [-7.44,-1.25]). --- Insert Figure 1 about here --- Sensory Task LMM showed that nor the main effect of Time (F(1,1546)=3.166, p=.075) or Body Part (F(1,1546)=0.736, p=.48), neither the interaction was significant (F(1,1546)=2.019, p=.133). For the sake of transparency Figure 2 reports all the condition averages and the related CIs. --- Insert Figure 2 about here --- Page 9 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version Attention Task LMM identified a significant main effect of Body Part (F(1,1020)=20.354, p<.001). The interaction between Body Part and Time fall in the grey area of a p-value that approaches the significant level without crossing it (F(1,1020)=3.354, p=.067). The main effect Time was not significant (F(1,1020=0.038, p=.846). The inspection of CI (Figure 3) shows that the blocks involving the elbow have a more proximal deviation than those involving the index finger (proximal: -7.48% [-11.11,-3.86]; distal: - 6.38% [-10.00,-2.76]). Crucially, when considering Time, we observed that attention to the proximal end tends to induce a more proximal shift (baseline proximal: -7.24% [-10.86,-3.61]; post-attention proximal: -7.73% [-11.36,-4.11]) than moving the attention to the distal end finger (baseline distal: -6.58% [-10.21,-2.96]; post-attention distal: -6.18% [-9.80,-2.56]) that instead reduce the proximal shift; however, it is important to consider these latter results cautiously considering the level of evidence. --- Insert Figure 3 about here --- Discussion Our findings showed that, at baseline, participants tended to perceive their forearm as shorter than its real length, as indicated by the proximal shift of the subjective midpoint. This pattern of data is in line with the studies in the literature showing that the representation of upper body parts is usually underestimated (e.g. for hands, see Longo, 2022; for arms, see Romano et al., 2019; Bolognini et al., 2012; Tosi et al., 2018; for face, see Mora et al., 2018). Although some exceptions have also been reported (Fuentes et al., 2013; Sadibolova et al., 2019 Linkenauger et al., 2015), especially when different aspects of body representation were explored or the use of a tool was required (e.g., Garbarini et al., 2015, Sposito et al., 2012). It has been suggested Page 10 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version that the pattern of under-over-estimation of body parts may be linked to the specific use of these body parts in everyday life (Ferretti, 2016; Caggiano & Cocchini, 2020; Caggiano et al., 2021), and distortions may be crucial for more efficient and adaptive motor control (Longo, 2022; Bassolino & Becchio, 2023). In our study, the same trend of underestimation was also observed after all three types of manipulation, suggesting a relatively stable underestimation of the representation of one’s own upper limb. However, the impact of each manipulation was different. In detail, the subjective midpoint deviation scores were significantly modulated by the Motor task but not by the Sensory one, whereas in the Attentional task, a close-to- significance interaction between Body Part and Time was observed. These results shed interesting light on the differential contribution of movement and attention in shaping subjective body metric changes. The Motor task induced an increment, compared to the baseline, of the proximal shift regardless of which body part was moved. At first glance, this seems to contradict evidence from one of our recent studies, which showed proximal shift when active motor training involved shoulder movements and distal shift when active training involved the use of the wrist and fingers (Romano et al., 2019). Critically, the effect observed in our previous study was interpreted as result of the crucial role of motor patterns in determining the direction of the perceived changes in body metric therefore, a possible explanation of the current findings, is that movement per se, when not goal-directed to any external target in the environment, tends to induce a shift toward the body (proximal); possibly because of the preponderant weight of proprioceptive information in non-goal-directed motor actions. The crucial aspect of goal- direction of purposeful actions was elegantly demonstrated in a study by D’Angelo et al. (2018). The authors showed how the agency of goal-directed movements could induce enlargement or contraction of body representation depending on the location of the target of Page 11 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version the action and agency. The lack of a purposeful goal toward an external target may have allowed the isolation of the impact of movement on the representation of one’s own body part. Focusing attention on a specific body part (i.e. hand or elbow) also seems to potentially moderate body representation even if the results suggest different implications from the Motor task. Although the effect has to be taken cautiously, in the Attention task, we observed a possible interaction effect which seems to suggest that the allocation of attention to the elbow is likely to lead to a further proximal shift, whereas the allocation of attention to the hand is likely to lead to a reduction of the proximal shift. It might be argued that the trend observed in this condition could be the effects of the ‘stimulation trials’ which intermingled with the ‘neutral trials’ (i.e., no stimulation). However, considering the absence of any effect in the Sensory task (where stimulations were delivered consistently across trials), we believe it is unlikely that a single supra-threshold stimulation drives the potential effect observed in the Attention task. Taking these findings together, they suggest that subjective elongation of the arm observed during tool-use might be the result of an embodiment of the motor characteristics of tool-use guided by an attention shift, towards the body part actively used during the use of the tool, that may trigger the incorporation of the tool (Romano & Maravita, 2021). It has been consistently shown that the modulation of body representation for action and perceived reachable space occurs when individuals use a tool that functionally increases action capabilities (Bourgeois et al. 2014; Patané et al. 2016, 2017). However, the modulation of our subjective body representation is also possible, though diminished, without the tool manipulation (e.g. Romano et al., 2019; Caggiano et al., 2021; Bassolino et al., 2015) suggesting that the presence of the tool is not, strictly speaking, necessary to induce a detectable bisection shift towards the body part manipulated. In light of these considerations, we need to reconsider the aforementioned point about the purpose of a movement. In virtually all studies investigating the malleability of Page 12 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version body schema, participants are asked to perform a movement in order to accomplish a task and, crucially, the internal focus of attention is directed not only to the specific body part but also to the goal-oriented motor patterns required to accomplish the task (Bruno et al., 2019). Indeed, rapid processes of internal concentration and automatic attentional shift on the tool (or the body part) have been shown to guide other sensory integrations in the perception of body representation and the direction of perceived body representation changes (Holmes, Calvert, et al., 2007; Holmes, Sanabria, et al., 2007; Reed & Park, 2021). Therefore, a shift in attention seems to be an important modulator of the body metrics change. However, the weak effect that we observed after the manipulation of attention seems to indicate that attention needs to be coupled with motor signals to induce a significant effect (Longo et al., 2010; Medina & Coslett, 2016; Romano et al., 2019). The lack of any effect of the Sensory task/manipulation on body representation is a bit surprising, but we cannot exclude a lack of sensitivity of our paradigm for this specific factor considering that the lack of evidence should not be confounded with the evidence of lack of effect in the frequentist inferential framework. Although, to our knowledge, gender has not been typically reported to be a significant factor modulating body metrics in similar experimental paradigms on healthy individuals (e.g Fuentes et al., 2013; Stone et al., 2018; D’Angelo et al., 2018, Bassolino et al., 2015), further studies may want to replicate these findings with the inclusion of male participants. Our findings are not conclusive, but they contribute to filling an important gap in the current literature and call for some reconceptualization of the empirical investigation of body representation. It has often been pointed out that body representation is susceptible to experimental task demand; hence, different experimental conditions may lead to different results (de Vignemont, 2010). Because the body representation is multimodal and extremely complex in its nature, this observation is not entirely surprising but poses a significant question in regard to how to overcome this Page 13 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version potential limitation. Often researchers have tackled the topic of body representation by devising studies that address a specific question with a specific experimental paradigm without necessarily considering the different contributions of cognitive and sensorimotor components that may have in the observed results. In the present study, we adopted a ‘comparative’ approach to isolate and disentangle the role of movement, somatosensation and attention directed towards the body on the modulation of perceived body metrics by isolating these factors from others. Such comparisons have highlighted that within the same experimental paradigm (i.e. the forearm bisection task), different manipulations produce different outcomes raising the question of what these differences tell us about the nature of body representation. Overall, the above considerations and the present results suggest that changes in body representation are not simply shaped by motor patterns or attentional factors. These two are not mutually exclusive, and their interaction might be crucial. Future studies should attempt to further elucidate the individual weight of these factors and how they may interact and drive body representation’s plasticity with and without the use of a tool. Declaration of conflicting interests The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Page 14 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version Data availability Readers seeking access to the datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study should contact the corresponding author. Access will be granted to named individuals. There are no further conditions. No part of the study procedures and analyses were pre-registered prior to the research being conducted. Authors’ contribution GC & DR formulation of hypotheses and paradigm, DR data analysis, DDS data collection and feedback on drafts; PC, GC, DR interpretation of findings and writing the manuscript. All authors provided relevant contributions to the study. Page 15 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version References Bassolino, M., & Becchio, C. (2023). The 'hand paradox': distorted representations guide optimal actions. Trends Cognitive Sciences, 27(1):7-8. Bassolino, M., Finisguerra, A., Canzoneri, E., Serino, A., & Pozzo, T. (2015). Dissociating effect of upper limb non-use and overuse on space and body representations. Neuropsychologia, 70, 385–392. Bassolino, M., Franza, M., Guanziroli, E., Sorrentino, G., Canzoneri, E., Colombo, M., ... Serino, A. (2022). Body and peripersonal space representations in chronic stroke patients with upper limb motor deficits. Brain Communications, 4(4):fcac179. Bolognini, N., Casanova, D., Maravita, A., Vallar, G. (2012) Bisecting real and fake body parts: effects of prism adaptation after right brain damage. Front Hum Neurosci, 6, 154. Bruno, V., Carpinella, I., Rabuffetti, M., De Giuli, L., Sinigaglia, C., Garbarini, F., Ferrarin, M. (2019). How Tool-Use Shapes Body Metric Representation: Evidence From Motor Training With and Without Robotic Assistance. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13: 299. Burrack, A, & Brugger, P. (2005). Individual differences in susceptibility to experimentally induced phantom sensations. Body Image, 2(3), 307–313. Canzoneri, E., Marzolla, M., Amoresano, A., Verni, G., Serino, A. (2013). Amputation and prosthesis implantation shape body and peripersonal space representations. Scientific Reports, 3:2844. Caggiano, P., Bertone, E., Cocchini, G. (2021). Same action in different spatial locations induces selective modulation of body metric representation. Experimental Brain Research, 239(8):2509-2518. Caggiano, P., Cocchini, G. (2020). The functional body: does body representation reflect functional properties? Experimental Brain Research, 238(1):153-169. Page 16 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version Caggiano, P., Veronelli, L., Mora, L., Arduino, L.S., & Cocchini, G. (2020). The downsized hand in personal neglect. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 42(10): 1072–1084 Cardinali L, Brozzoli C, Urquizar C, Salemme R, Roy AC, Farnè A. (2011). When action is not enough: tool-use reveals tactile-dependent access to Body Schema. Neuropsychologia, Cardinali, L., Brozzoli, C., Finos, L., Roy, A. C., Farnè, A. (2016). The rules of tool incorporation: Tool morpho-functional & sensori-motor constraints. Cognition, 149, 1–5. 49(13):3750-7. Cardinali, L., Frassinetti, F., Brozzoli, C., Urquizar, C., Roy, A.C. Farnè A. (2009). Tool-use induces morphological updating of the body schema. Current Biology, 19(12), R478–R479. Cohen, J. (1990). Things I have learned (so far). American Psychologist, 45(12):1304–1312. Cohen, J. (1994). The earth is round (p < 0.05). American Psychologist, 49(12):997–1003 D'Angelo, M., di Pellegrino, G., Seriani, S., Gallina, P., Frassinetti, F. (2018). The sense of agency shapes body schema and peripersonal space. Scientific Report, 8(1), 13847. de Vignemont, F. (2010). Body schema and body image—Pros and cons. Neuropsychologia 48(3), 669–680. de Vignemont, F., Ehrsson, H.H., and Haggard, P. (2005). Bodily illusions modulate tactile perception. Current Biology, 15, 1286–1290. Farnè A, Iriki A, Làdavas E. (2005). Shaping multisensory action-space with tools: evidence from patients with cross-modal extinction. Neuropsychologia;43(2):238-48. Farnè A, Làdavas E. (2000). Dynamic size-change of hand peripersonal space following tool use. Neuroreport 5;11(8):1645-9. Farnè A, Serino A, Làdavas E. (2007). Dynamic size-change of peri-hand space following tool- use: determinants and spatial characteristics revealed through cross-modal extinction. Cortex;43(3):436-43. Page 17 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version Ferretti, G. (2016) Through the forest of motor representations. Conscious Cogn 43:177–196. Francis, S.T., Kelly, E.F., Bowtell, R., Dunseath, W.J.R., Folger, S.E., McGlone, F. (2000). fMRI of the Responses to Vibratory Stimulation of Digit Tips. NeuroImage, 11(3), 188-202. Fuentes, C.T., Longo, M.R., Haggard, P. (2013). Body image distortions in healthy adults. Acta Psychologica, 144 344–351. Galigani, M., Castellani, N., Donno, B., Franza, M., Zuber, C., Allet, L., Garbarini, F., Bassolino, M. (2020). Effect of tool-use observation on metric body representation and peripersonal space. Neuropsychologia, 148, 107622. Gallagher, S. (1986). Body image and body schema: A conceptual clarification. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 7, 541–554. Gallagher, S. (2005). How the body shapes the mind. New York: Oxford University Press. Garbarini ,F., Fossataro, C., Berti, A., Gindri, P., Romano, D., Pia, L., della Gatta, F., Maravita, A., Neppi-Modona, M. (2015). When your arm becomes mine: pathological embodiment of alien limbs using tools modulates own body representation. Neuropsychologia, ;70:402-13. Holmes NP, Calvert GA, Spence C. Tool use changes multisensory interactions in seconds: evidence from the crossmodal congruency task. Exp Brain Res. 2007b Dec;183(4):465-76. Holmes NP, Sanabria D, Calvert GA, Spence C. (2007). Tool-use: capturing multisensory spatial attention or extending multisensory peripersonal space? Cortex, 43(3):469-89. Iriki A, Tanaka M, Iwamura Y (1996). Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. Neuroreport 7(14): 2325—2330. Lackner, J.R. (1988). Some proprioceptive influences on the perceptual representation of body shape and orientation. Brain, 111(2), 281–297. Linkenauger, S.A., Wong, H.Y., Geuss, M., Stefanucci, J.K., McCulloch, K.C., Bülthoff, H.H., Mohler, B.J., Proffitt, D.R. (2015). The perceptual homunculus: the perception of the Page 18 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version relative proportions of the human body. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 144(1):103-13. Longo, M. R., Haggard, P. (2010). An implicit body representation underlying human position sense. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 11727–11732. Longo, M. R. (2022). Distortion of mental body representations Trends in Cognitive. Sciences, 26, pp. 241-254. Maravita A, Husain M, Clarke K, Driver J. (2001). Reaching with a tool extends visual-tactile interactions into far space: evidence from cross-modal extinction. Neuropsychologia, 39(6):580-5. Maravita A, Iriki A. (2004). Tools for the body (schema). Trends Cognitive Sciences, 8(2):79- 86. Maravita A, Romano D. (2018). The parietal lobe and tool use. In Vallar, G., Coslett, H. B (Eds), Handbook of Clinical Neurology (vol. 151, pp. 481-498). Elsevier. Maravita A, Spence C, Driver J. (2003). Multisensory integration and the body schema: close to hand and within reach. Current Biology, 13(13):R531-9. Maravita A, Spence C, Kennett S, Driver J. (2002). Tool-use changes multimodal spatial interactions between vision and touch in normal humans. Cognition, 83(2):B25-34. Martel M, Cardinali L, Roy AC, Farnè A. (2016). Tool-use: An open window into body representation and its plasticity. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 33(1-2):82-101. Martel M, Finos L, Koun E, Farnè A, Roy A.C. (2021). The long developmental trajectory of body representation plasticity following tool use. Science Reports, 11(1):559. Masson, M.E.J., Loftus, G.R (2003) Using confidence intervals for graphically based data interpretation. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57(3):203–220. Page 19 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version Medina, J., Coslett, H.B. (2010). From maps to form to space: touch and the body schema. Neuropsychologia, 48(3):645-54. Mora L, Gonzalez Alted C, Cocchini G. (2023). The flubbed body: Pathological body size representation in personal neglect. Neuropsychologia, 183:108522. Mora L, Cowie D, Banissy MJ, Cocchini G. (2018). My true face: Unmasking one's own face representation. Acta Psychologica (Amst), 191:63-68. Mora L, Sedda A, Esteban T, Cocchini G. (2021). The signing body: extensive sign language practice shapes the size of hands and face. Experimental Brain Research, 239(7):2233-2249. Mora, L., Cowie, D., Banissy, M.J., Cocchini, G. (2018). My true face: Unmasking one's own face representation. Acta Psychologica, 191, 63-68. Purcell, J.R., Chen, J., Moussa-Tooks, A.B., Hetrick, W.P. (2020). Psychometric evaluation of the Pinocchio Illusion Questionnaire. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82(5):2728- 2737. Rognini, G., Petrini, F.M., Raspopovic, S., Valle, G., Granata, G., Strauss, I., ... Blanke, O. (2019). Multisensory bionic limb to achieve prosthesis embodiment and reduce distorted phantom limb perceptions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 90(7):833- 836. Romano, D. Uberti, E., Caggiano, P., Cocchini, G., Maravita, A. (2019). Different tool training induces specific effects on body metric representation. Experimental Brain Research, 237(2), 493-501. Romano, D., Maravita, A. (2021). Plasticity and tool use in the body schema. Body Schema and Body Image: New Directions, 117. Sadibolova, R., Ferrè E.R., Linkenauger S.A., Longo, M.R. (2019). Distortions of perceived volume and length of body parts. Cortex, 111, 74-86. Page 20 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version Sato, Y., Kawase, T., Takano, K., Spence, C., Kansaku, K. (2017). Incorporation of prosthetic limbs into the body representation of amputees: Evidence from the crossed hands temporal order illusion. Progress in Brain Research, 236:225-241. Sorrentino, G., Franza, M., Zuber, C., Blanke, O., Serino, A., Bassolino, M.(2021). How ageing shapes body and space representations: A comparison study between healthy young and older adults. Cortex, 136:56-76. Sposito, A., Bolognini, N., Vallar, G., Maravita, A. (2012). Extension of perceived arm length following tool-use: clues to plasticity of body metrics. Neuropsychologia 50(9), 2187–2194. Sposito, A. V, Bolognini, N., Vallar, G., Posteraro, L., & Maravita, A. (2010). The spatial encoding of body parts in patients with neglect and neurologically unimpaired participants. Neuropsychologia, 48(1), 334–340. Tosi G, Romano D, Maravita A. (2018). Mirror Box Training in Hemiplegic Stroke Patients Affects Body Representation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4;11:617. Tosi, G., Romano, D. (2020). The longer the reference, the shorter the legs: How response modality affects body perception. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82(7), 3737- 3749. Tosi, G., Maravita, A., Romano, D. (2022). I am the metre: The representation of one’s body size affects the perception of tactile distances on the body. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75(4), 583-597. Page 21 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version Figure Captions Figure 1. Results from the Motor task Bars indicate 95% confidence interval Figure 2. Results from the Sensory task Bars indicate 95% confidence interval Figure 3. Results from the Attention task Bars indicate 95% confidence interval Page 22 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version Proximal Distal -12 -10 -8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 Baseline Post Movement Su bj ec tiv e M id po in t D ev ia tio n % <- - p ro xi m al / di st al -- > Figure 1. Results from the Motor task Bars indicate 95% confidence interval Page 23 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version Proximal Distal Double -12 -10 -8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 Baseline Post Su bj ec tiv e M id po in t D ev ia tio n % <- - p ro xi m al / di st al -- > Sensory stimulation Figure 2. Results from the Sensory task Bars indicate 95% confidence interval Page 24 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript Peer Review Version Proximal Distal -12 -10 -8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 Baseline Post Su bj ec tiv e M id po in t D ev ia tio n % <- - p ro xi m al / di st al -- > Attention allocation Figure 3. Results from the Attention task Bars indicate 95% confidence interval Page 25 of 25 Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187385 Author Accepted Manuscript