Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorMurphy, V.
dc.contributor.authorHayes, J.
dc.date.accessioned2011-03-07T11:10:59Z
dc.date.available2011-03-07T11:10:59Z
dc.date.issued2010
dc.identifier.citationMurphy , V & Hayes , J 2010 , ' Processing English compounds in the first and second language : the influence of the middle morpheme ' , Language Learning , vol. 60 , no. 1 , pp. 194-220 . https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2009.00555.x
dc.identifier.issn0023-8333
dc.identifier.otherPURE: 195339
dc.identifier.otherPURE UUID: 17dbf99c-49ef-464b-9e95-a30735772824
dc.identifier.otherdspace: 2299/5433
dc.identifier.otherScopus: 77950112828
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/2299/5433
dc.descriptionThe definitive version is available at www3.interscience.wiley.com Copyright Wiley [Full text of this article is not available in the UHRA]
dc.description.abstractNative English speakers tend to exclude regular plural inflection when producing English noun-noun compounds (e.g., rat-eater not rats-eater) while allowing irregular plural inflection within compounds (e.g., mice-eater) (Clahsen, 1995; Gordon, 1985; Hayes, Smith & Murphy, 2005; Lardiere, 1995; Murphy, 2000). Exposure to the input alone has been considered insufficient to explain this dissociation between regular and irregular plurals in compounds because naturally occurring compounds in English rarely have plurals of any type included within them (e.g., Gordon, 1985). However, the constraint on the production of plural inflection in English compounds could be derived from the patterns in which regular plural and possessive morphemes occur in the input. To explore this idea, native adult English speakers and adult Chinese learners of English were asked to process a series of compounds containing different medial morphemes and phonemes. Comparisons were made across compounds with regular and irregular plurals and possessive [-s]. Native speakers (NS) of English processed compounds with medial possessive morphology faster than compounds with medial regular plural morphology. The second language learners did not show the same pattern as the NSs, which could be due to the fact that they had considerably less exposure to the relevant input patterns relative to the NSs. Regular plurals may be excluded before a rightmost noun in English because the pattern "Noun–[-s] morpheme–Noun" is more frequently used for marking possession in English. Irregular plurals do not end in the [-s] morpheme and therefore do not "compete" with the possessive marker and, consequently, may be optionally included in compounds. It is possible, therefore, that the input English learners receive could indeed be sufficient to constrain this aspect of English compound production.en
dc.language.isoeng
dc.relation.ispartofLanguage Learning
dc.subjectcompounding
dc.subjectinflectional morphology
dc.subjectpossessive morphology
dc.titleProcessing English compounds in the first and second language : the influence of the middle morphemeen
dc.contributor.institutionDepartment of Psychology
dc.description.statusPeer reviewed
rioxxterms.versionofrecordhttps://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2009.00555.x
rioxxterms.typeJournal Article/Review
herts.preservation.rarelyaccessedtrue


Files in this item

FilesSizeFormatView

There are no files associated with this item.

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record